|
1978 IPS Perimetry Standards1. Definitions of Perimetry and of the Visual Field Perimetry is the measurement of visual functions of the eye at topographically defined loci in the visual field. The visual field is that portion of the external environment of the observer wherein the steadily fixating eye(s) can detect visual stimuli. 2. Need for Specifications and Tolerances The fundamental purpose of standardization is to provide a common framework for measurement. This allows exchange and comparison of information obtained at different times and in different places. If a common measurement scheme can be achieved, then development upon that base can proceed in an orderly manner. In perimetry few standards exist and certain of these are imperfectly specified. This situation needs to be rectified. Specification also implies consideration of tolerances. Tolerances include instrument setting accuracy as well as measurement accuracy. Because tolerances in a clinical office are not comparable with those achievable in a research laboratory, an effort will be made to set standards which define conditions where small errors do not significantly alter results or interpretations of data. 3. Applicability of this Standard This standard is written for all individuals engaged in perimetry and especially for clinicians for use in their offices, departments and clinics. It is directed also towards the manufacturer who provides visual field test equipment. These standards also set minimum criteria for reporting research results. The goal is to set a reasonable minimum set of standards for testing of the visual field. Note that different requirements or strategies may be needed for different tasks. 4. Specifications of Magnitudes and Units This committee makes use of the International System of Units published by the International Bureau of Weights and Measures (Le Systeme International d'Unites, 1970, OFFILIB, 48 Rue Gay-Lussac, F-75005, Paris, France, Revised edition 1977). See also: The International System of Units, NBS Special Publication 330, 1977, US Department of Commerce, National Bureau of Standards, US Government Printing Office, Washington DC 20402 (SD Catalogue No. C13.10:330/4). Supplemental use is made of the Vocabulary of the Commission Internationale de l'Eclairage (International Lighting Vocabulary of the Commission Internationale de l'Eclairage, 3rd ed;, 1970, Bureau Central de la CIE, 4 Avenue du Recteur Poincare, F-75016, Paris, France). In the Proceedings (Acta) of the XXIInd International Ophthalmological Congress, Paris, 1974, p78 and 93, the Concilium Ophthalmologicum Universale published recommendations regarding the use of the International System of Units in ophthalmological practice. There is on record an earlier international standard on perimetry which was published by the XIII Concilium Ophthalmologicum 1929, Hollandia, NV Boek- en Steendrukkerij, Edward Ijdo, Leiden, 9 pages. The present document is meant to supercede this earlier standard. Recently the Committee on Vision, the National Research Council, National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America published the "First Interprofessional Standard for Visual Field Testing," National Academy of Sciences, Washington, DC, 1975. Certain aspects of the present document are based upon this publication. 5. Photometric Specification For proper control of visual stimuli in perimetry, provision of one or more light sources is necessary. That is, the background, test stimuli, and all supplementary targets or display fields need to be specified photometrically. The specification of visual stimuli is complex. Properly, radiant energy determinations should be made, followed by suitable luminous conversions for different field areas and stimulus conditions. For most practical clinical situations the IPS recommends that the visual stimulus in perimetry be specified in luminance units measured at the center of the entrance pupil of the eye. The visual stimulus is essentially defined by this luminance, the direction of the stimulus in the field of view and the area of the entrance pupil of the eye. Since we often cannot control pupil size in the clinical environment, the least we can achieve is to specify luminance at the center of the entrance pupil of the eye, and to request the examiner to record this luminance and the entrance pupil size at the time of the measurement. In certain conditions special additional calibration requirements exist, e.g. for short duration or coloured stimuli. We ask the manufacturer to specify the operating conditions of his instrument. ideally this would include the complete specifications of lamps, filters (including transmission curves), and desired operating conditions. Luminance at the center of the entrance pupil of the eye should be specified for defined operating conditions of properly centred light sources and associated optics. Similarly, the spectral distribution at the entrance pupil of the eye should be defined. In addition, definition of desired operating colour temperature and CIE co-ordinates is highly desirable. A simple scheme for assuring that the instrument is functioning within reasonable tolerances of these specified values should be provided. Included would be some test of luminance and/or indication for replacement of light sources. The international unit of luminance is the candela per meter squared, cd/m2 or cd.m-2. Other units are now regarded as obsolete. Although strictly speaking not the same units2, conversion to apostilb and millilambert values can be made using the following relationships: 10/p candela/m2 = 1 millilambert = 10 apostilbs, where 10/p = 3.183 (approximate) While this group would prefer luminance measurement of perimetric devices by objective small field test instruments, an acceptable alternative would be to provide a measure convertible into luminance at the center of the entrance pupil of the eye. 6. Background or Adapting Luminance A. Specification of luminance For routine perimetric instruments used in clinical offices it is recommended that a value of background luminance be chosen such that it is photopic and it falls within that range of background luminances over which the Weber fraction remains constant, ie, DL/LB = constant.3 DL is defined as the just detectable luminance difference between test target and background,4 and LB is background luminance (also see section on contrast). The proposed background level is generally higher than that found in perimeters in use today. This setting criterion is recommended because (a) it requires less sensitive calibration equipment, (b) it is less sensitive to modest fluctuations (or changes) in light source output, (c) the result is less dependent upon modest variations in eye pupil size, (d) visual functions are tested at clearly defined photopic levels, and (e) fixation control is easier than at low luminance adaptation levels. If this background luminance cannot be achieved, it is recommended that for routine office purposes no less than 10 candelas/m2 be used. A background luminance of 10 candelas/m2 is below, but near the level where DL/LB = constant over an extended range of values. Other light or adaptation levels offer advantages. Lower levels may provide an extended range of contrast values for testing, cataract patients may be better evaluated, and rod anomalies may be more effectively studied, etc. Thus, where adequate calibration capability exists and careful studies are conducted to rule out loss of confidence due to increased measurement variance, lower photopic or mesopic background luminance levels can serve a useful purpose. Similarly, higher background luminance values can be useful in test of the visual fatigue factor or for the development of colour perimetric tests. Thus the IPS recommends that instruments be constructed to be capable of calibration over a range of values. The IPS suggests that the standard be a minimum test condition rather than a limiting condition. We encourage careful research on this rather complex and crucial set of questions. We recognise that stability of determinations in special disease conditions, e.g., glaucoma, cataract, chorioretinal degenerations, optic neuropathies, etc., may require use of special or specific background luminance levels and special purpose instruments suitable for advanced diagnostic laboratories. 2 See discussion in the recently published US Standard relative to this point. 3 For example, see E Aulhorn, H Harms, and M Raabe, Documenta Ophthal. 20, 538-556, 1966; and J Enoch, Physiology (Chapter 3, pp 202-289) in A Sorsby, Modern Ophthalmology, Vol I, First Ed, 1963; and the recent USA Standard referenced above. 4 The specification of DL is somewhat arbitrary, because the probability of detecting the test target varies between 0 and 1 over a small range of luminances. DL is commonly specified as the luminance increment or difference corresponding to a detection probability of 0.5 (50% frequency-of-seeing). B. Preadaptation conditions It is highly desirable that the patient be adapted to the luminance of the background field before commencement of the perimetric test. A longer time period of preadaptation to this field is necessary for lower background luminance levels. It should also be longer if the patient enters the examination chamber from an intensely luminous environment. It is desirable that the manufacturer and examiner determine the light adaptation period which provides relatively stable response for the instrument and conditions used. Preadaptation conditions can also be important when testing individuals manifesting certain types of pathology. C. Diffusely reflecting surface It is desirable that the background field be a diffusely reflecting surface, ie, a non-glossy surface which at least approximates Lambert's Law. 7. Specification of the location of an Object in the Visual Field A polar co-ordinate system should be used when defining (a) the half-meridian and (b) the eccentricity of the center of the test target, both expressed in degrees. The zero degree half-meridian is defined to the right of the patient (as seen by the patient). The specified half-meridian then proceeds counterclockwise through 360 degrees about the fixation target (as seen by the patient). The fixation point is defined at having zero degree eccentricity. This assumes the patient has normal fixation. This system does not allow fine specification of the area of a scotoma or of an isopter because of non-linearity of representation,5 except in the case of a hemisphere where proportional solid angles are present. This requires the center of the entrance pupil of the eye to lie at the center of curvature of the hemisphere. In this special case, two equal solid angles located at different loci in the visual field subtend equal areas on the surface of the hemisphere. This condition does not exist if a flat test surface is used for examination of the visual field, e.g., when a tangent screen is employed. The same statement may be applied to the cartographic deformation of the field as expressed on a flat sheet of paper. There exist cartographic projections which attempt to represent areas proportionately. It is highly desirable to keep the tolerances for location, registration, and replication of test object position within narrow limits. If this cannot be achieved, the reliability of subsequent determinations is limited, especially for static (target not moving) perimetry. Accuracy is limited by the size and nature of the fixation target, the stability of patient fixation, and the mechanical capabilities of the perimetric device.6 In turn, these factors influence the selection of the smallest useful target size. Optimal fixation targets have not yet been defined. This is an important question which needs clarification through research. Obviously, it is desirable to monitor patient fixation directly. When central vision is impaired, special fixation targets or displays are often needed. It is desirable that measured test points should be indicated on the test record in an obvious manner. Clearly the more points tested, the better the characterisation of the visual field. The more repetitions of evaluations made at a single point, the greater the reliability of the determination. It is desirable that one or more points be evaluated more than once in order to define the approximate reliability of the test. It is desirable that interpolation or analysis techniques employed be clearly defined 5 Distinguish between ability to specify a location and an area. 6 Lens factors also influence accuracy of location and re-location of a target in the visual field. Apparent location of a target is influenced by power and centration of the lens correction, vertex distance, base curve and lens thickness. It is desirable that the lens(es) used and the vertex distance be noted. There is an advantage in keeping vertex distance small. 8. Target Specification (non-photometric) A. Size, distance, and form Ideally target dimensions should be specified in terms of the solid angle subtended at the centre of the entrance pupil of the eye and measured in steradians. Practically, this is not done, nor do we recommend such designation as essential at this time. A conceptually simpler scheme is the specification of the diameter of the target in terms of visual angle subtended at the center of the entrance pupil of the eye. This assumes that a round target is located at the point of fixation. If the target is not round, the diameter of the equivalent round target subtending the same area at the point of fixation may be used. Target diameters should be expressed in degrees, minutes, and seconds of arc. It is highly desirable to specify test target distance from the eye, because luminance is dependent on test distance for perimetric test targets of small dimension. (The same is not true for extended background fields.) Thus, for proper specification, it is highly desirable that both angular subtense and target distance be specified. Other factors, such as image blur resulting from several causes, also make specification of target distance desirable (see below, Image sharpness). As an example of proper specification, a target may subtend 6' of arc (angular diameter) at a 330 millimeter test distance. Alternatively, specification of the tangent of the angle subtended at the center of the entrance pupil of the eye for a target located at the fixation point has been widely used. DIAGRAM Fig. 1. d/D = 2 tan ( /2) This measure is expressed as a fraction, the diameter (d) of the target in millimeters divided by the distance of the fixation point from the eye (D) in millimeters (e.g. 2 mm white/1000mm). Of the two schemes (the angle subtended plus the test distance versus the tangent fraction ) the angle subtended and test distance is the preferred form of representation because it is conceptually simpler to compare angular subtense of targets on two different test instruments. Thus the IPS recommends the specification of equivalent7 test target diameter and distance as follows: (a) minutes of arc diameter at (b) millimeters test distance. In publications this form is preferred. Other systems may be used if a logical scheme is formulated and if conversion to the above preferred form is provided. When a tangent screen is used, and targets are displayed from the point of fixation, the angular subtenses, the solid angle subtended by the target, and the effective test distance are all altered. If a projector is used with a tangent screen, its center of projection must effectively be placed as near as possible to the location of the test eye in order to minimize distorting effects and blur. When tangent screens are used (or other perimetric devices) every effort should be made to make background luminance as uniform as possible. It is desirable that a number of different size targets be available for testing, and that the visual angles subtended by these targets extend from an effective point source to large targets in an orderly series. It is desirable that the target shape or form used be described. For targets that depart meaningfully from round or near round, it is desirable that orientation be indicated as well as its shape or form. 6 Lens factors also influence accuracy of location and re-location of a target in the visual field. Apparent location of a target is influenced by power and centration of the lens correction, vertex distance, base curve and lens thickness. It is desirable that the len(es) used and the vertex distance be noted. There is an advantage in keeping vertex distance small. 7 The use of equivalence is only valid for targets which approximate round shapes. B. Contrast The contrast of the test target against the background field may be represented in various ways, depending upon usage. Let LT = luminance of the test target, and LB = luminance of the background or adapting field,8 then DL is the just detectable change in target luminance, and would be defined as DL = LT - LB at threshold. A contrast may be positive or negative, that is, the target may have a higher luminance than the background (positive contrast) or the darker than the background (negative contrast). All of the following formats have been used to describe contrast = C: (a1) C = LT - LB (Recommended) ______ LB (a2) C = LT - LB 9 __________ LB (a3) CT = DL CT = contrast at threshold (Recommended) __ LB (b) C = LT _____ LB (c) C = LT - LB = LT - LB where (LT + LB) / 2 = mean luminance ___________ _________ LT + LB 2 {(LT + LB) / 2} NOTE: In a projection perimeter, DL is the projected incremental field and the luminance at the pointed tested LT = DL + LB. Negative contrast, ie, a darker target against a brighter background, is rarely used in perimetry, but is commonly used in conjunction with visual acuity charts. DIAGRAM Fig. 2. In routine perimetry form a1 or a3 is the recommended usage. For simplicity of design most tests performed use positive contrast. Form c is often used as a description of modulation in contrast sensitivity functions. Form a3 is also known as the Weber fraction. As the increment or contrast threshold, form a1 is equivalent to a3. Since many forms exist for expression of contrast, it is desirable that the form employed be indicated. C. Duration of Presentation 1. Non-moving or static target(s) (here we only consider a single presentation of the test target). The response of the visual system changes with duration of exposure of a visual stimulus. The exposure time at which this transition occurs is known as the critical duration. The critical duration is about 100 milliseconds, and varies with several factors, including test target locus in the field, target size, background field luminance and pathology. 8 That which follows assumes that LB is greater than zero. 9 |c| symbol denotes absolute value, ie, a value without sign. For exposures shorter than the critical duration, DL x Duration of exposure = Constant while for durations of exposure longer than the critical duration, DL = Constant Obviously the latter is a less demanding test situation for calibration as one less parameter needs to be specified in the test instrument. Shorter durations may be advantaged, but adequate calibration capability is advisable. If the duration of exposure exceeds the latency for a saccadic eye movement (approximately 250 milliseconds), there is a tendency for some patients to avert their eyes from the fixation point to look at the target. It is desirable that duration of exposure provided by the manufacturer be specified and that some scheme be available to determine whether a mechanical shutter or test flash device is operating properly if the duration of exposure is less than the critical duration. One must use care when presenting serial stimuli at the same test locus. It is desirable that the prior presentation shall not affect response to the later ones. 2. Moving or kinetic targets. If the target or stimulus is moved, as in kinetic perimetry, the most important point is the stability of rate of movement, ie, a fixed angular velocity, initially and at the time of re-examination. Some perimetrists use a different strategy, e.g., a somewhat slower rate of movement, in the central field. In most instances the target is moved from non-seeing to seeing. Other strategies may be used to fit specific needs. In recording and/or reporting results it is desirable (to the extent possible) that test conditions employed be described. Detectability of a moving target is dependent upon test target luminance and/or contrast, area, direction, and rate of movement. The measured results are subject to meaningful variation if such factors are not properly controlled. The specification of optimal test conditions is a complex question requiring further research. Thus, at this time, the IPS does not recommend any single desired rate of movement or test strategy. In so stating, the IPS in no way means to under-estimate the importance of kinetic perimetry. D. Image sharpness One of the least appreciated variables in visual field testing is the blur of the retinal image of the test target. Many factors affect image blur. Appropriate optical correction to the test distance is needed especially for small test targets. This correction will vary with presbyopia, the use of miotics, cycloplegics, and in the presence of many forms of pathology, etc. 9. Color Perimetry When reporting data, it is highly desirable to specify the observer's task, whether it be just detection or a judgement of hue and saturation of the target. In colour perimetry we recommend that both target and background field radiance and luminance be specified at the center of the entrance pupil of the eye. In anticipation of the development of new colour perimetric tests, the IPS recommends a general increase in background and target luminance levels; spectral specification in the plane of the entrance pupil of light sources and stimuli (including the properties of filters); and, if possible, designation of CIE co-ordinates of the same elements.10 Further, definition of colour temperature of the source and a logical scheme for replacement of aged light sources has been recommended above. For colour testing it is preferable to use nearly monochromatic stimuli as this greatly simplifies calibration requirements. Similarly (and particularly when non-monochromatic light stimuli are used) the use of light sources which emit continuous spectra simplifies analysis of stimuli. 10. Other Factors A. Attention signal and shutter noise In many applications of perimetry it is useful to provide a signal or cue to indicate that a stimulus is about to be presented. Such cues are often auditory. This clearly influences the probability of response and in certain situations may be more effective. Similarly, for long duration stimuli, the noise of an activated shutter can serve the same purpose. B. Distractions to be avoided It is recommended that the perimeter be placed in a quiet room where light conditions can be completely controlled, and distractions can be avoided. C. Relative and absolute scotomas It is important to differentiate between relative and absolute scotomas. A relative scotomas is defined as a partial visual deficit in a given area of visual field. An absolute scotoma implies total loss of vision in a given field area. Practically, absolute scotomas are usually defined in terms of the largest, most intense target available to the perimetrist. In fact, some response may yet remain and may have been revealed if still larger or more intense targets had been used (assuming stray light effects have been considered). Thus, it is desirable, in discussions of absolute scotomas, to specify the largest and most intense stimulus employed. 11. Acceptance and Revision of these Standards a. The proposed standards have been approved by the R.G. on Standards and the Board of the IPS. b. These Standards, once approved, will remain in force until revised by the R.G. on Standards of the IPS. These Standards must be reviewed every four years and either reaffirmed, modified, or replaced. c. The R.G. on Standards stands ready to provide reasonable advice, and to offer clarification relative to matters contained in this set of standards. All correspondence relative to such matters and suggested improvements should be directed to the Secretary of the IPS. 10 It should be recognized that CIE coordinates as specified for central vision may not be valid for peripheral field test points. IPS English A0 Stimulation A1 Inadequate stimulus A2 Adequate stimulus A3 Distal stimulus A4 Proximal stimulus A5 Threshold stimulus A6 Subthreshold stimulus A7 Suprathreshold stimulus A8 Radiation A9 Complex radiation A10 Monochromatic radiation A11 Wavelength - l A12 Nanometer - nm A13 Spectral distribution A14 Colour (or) A15 Colour (or) temperature A16 Kelvin - K A17 Dominant wavelength - ld A18 Excitation purity pe A19 Chromaticity coordinates - x, y; x10, y10 A20 Standard illuminant - A, B, C, D65 A21 Complementary colour (or)s A22 Radiance - Le A23 Watt per steradian per square metre (er) - W.sr-1 .m-2 A24 Luminance - L A25 Candela per square metre (er) - cd.m-2 A26 Irradiance - Ee A27 Watt per square metre (er) - W.m-2 A28 Illuminance - E A29 Lux - lx A30 Reflection A31 Specular reflection A32 Diffuse reflection A33 Uniform diffuse reflection A34 Mixed reflection A35 Regular reflectance - pr A36 Diffuse reflectance - pd A37 Gloss A38 Transmission A39 Regular transmission A40 Diffuse transmission A41 Uniform diffuse transmission A42 Mixed transmission A43 Regular transmittance - tr A44 Diffuse transmittance - tr A45 Absorption A46 Absorptance - a A47 Optical density - D A48 Diffusion A49 Refraction A50 Dispersion A51 Diffraction A52 Polarized light A53 Unpolarized light A54 Coherent light A55 Incoherent light A56 Temporal modulation A57 Intermittent stimulation A58 Pulsed stimulation A59 Periodic pulsed stimulation A60 Period A61 Frequency - v A62 Hertz - Hz A63 Duty cycle, light dark ratio A64 Sinusoidally varying stimulation A65 Modulation depth A66 Spatial modulation A67 Modulation transfer function - MTF A68 Interferometric resolution A69 Object, target (=O) A70 Background (=Bd) A71 Surround (=Sd) A72 (O, Bd, Sd) shape A73 round A74 elliptical A75 square A76 (O, Bd, Sd) contour A77 Edge gradient A78 (O, Bd, Sd) distance -l A79 (O, Bd, Sd) diameter -d A80 Millimetre (er) - mm A81 (O, Bd, Sd) visual angle - q A82 Minute of arc - ' A83 Second of arc - " A84 (O, Bd,Sd) area -S A85 Square millimetre (er) -mm2 A86 (O, Bd, Sd) solid angle - w A87 Steradian -sr A88 (O, Bd, Sd) luminance - L A89 Decibel - dB A90 (O) intensity - I A91 Candela - cd A92 (O, Bd, Sd) colour (or) A93 red A94 orange A95 yellow A96 green A97 blue A98 violet A100 white A101 grey A102 black A103 (O, Bd, Sd) Munsell notation A104 (O, Bd, Sd) exposure duration - t A105 Second of time -s A106 (O) angular velocity A107 Degree per second A108 Rate of increase of (O, Bd, Sd) luminance A109 (Photometric) luminance contrast - C = DL/L A110 Corneal illuminance -Ecor A111 Luminance measured from the position of the centre (er) of the entrance pupil - Lpup A112 Pupil diameter -d pup A113 Pupil area - S pup A114 Retinal illuminance - Eret A115 Troland - td A116 Reduced troland - tdr A117 Blur of the retinal image A118 Intraocular stray light A119 Equivalent veiling luminance B0 Perception B1 Brightness B2 Lightness B3 Hue B4 Saturation B5 Chromaticity B6 Bezold-Brucke phenomenon B7 Flicker B8 Stroboscopic effect B9 Speed of perception B10 Subjective colour (or)s B11 Fusion frequency - FF B12 Local adaptation B13 Discomfort glare B14 Disability glare B15 Adaptation B16 Photopic vision B17 Mesopic vision B18 Scotopic vision B19 Adaptation curve B20 Break (in adaptation curve) B21 Chromatic adaptation B22 Threshold B23 Sensitivity B24 Absolute threshold B25 Absolute sensitivity B26 Difference (or increment) threshold - DL B27 Difference (or increment) sensitivity - 1/DL B28 Perceived contrast B29 Luminosity contrast B30 Colour (or) contrast B31 Simultaneous contrast B32 Successive contrast B33 Contrast threshold (=Weber fraction) - DL/L B34 Contrast sensitivity L/DL B35 Modulation threshold - DL/S(L1 + L2) B36 Liminal brightness increment (UK) - Just noticeable difference (US)- j.n.d. B37 Visual resolution B38 Visual acuity B39 Stereoscopic visual acuity B40 Kinetic (=dynamic) visual acuity B41 Achromatic threshold B42 Chromatic threshold B43 Photochromatic interval B44 Spatial summation B45 Successive lateral spatial summation B46 Receptive field B47 Temporal summation B48 Summation exponent B49 Summation number B50 Critical duration B51 Inhibition B52 Sensitization B53 Sustained-type visual response B54 Westheimer function B55 Transient-type visual response B56 Rivalry in the visual field B57 Binocular rivalry B58 Spectral relative luminous efficiency function - V(l) B59 Purkinje phenomenon B60 Stiles' p function B61 Directional sensitivity function (= Stiles-Crawford effect) B62 Entoptic phenomenon B63 Maxwell's spot B64 Haidinger's brushes B65 Directional selectivity B66 Reaction time B67 Optokinetic nystagmus B68 Attention B69 Breadth of attention B70 Expectancy B71 Conspicuousness B72 Distraction B73 Fatigue B74 Mental processing block B75 Visual performance C0 Technique C1 Psycho-physical method C2 Perimetry C3 Campimetry C4 Screening method C5 Confrontation test C6 Scotometer C7 Plate for evaluating scotomas C8 Tangent screen C9 Angioscotometer C10 Perimetric arc C11 Portable hand perimeter C12 Hemispheric (=cupola, = bowl) perimeter C13 Projection perimeter C14 Monocular perimetry C15 Binocular perimetry with fusional stimulus C16 Participation binocular perimetry C17 Phase difference haploscopy C18 Measurement of peripheral visual acuity C19 Werblin's rotating windmill pattern C20 Colour (or) perimetry C21 Flicker perimetry C22 Photopic perimetry C23 Mesopic perimetry C24 Scotopic perimetry C25 Adaptoperimetry C26 Temporal adaptoperimetry C27 Steady-state adaptoperimetry C28 Fundus image-controlled perimetry C29 Combined method (=check-up) C30 Subjective method C31 Entoptic method C32 Objective method C33 Pupillomotor perimetry C34 Optokinetic perimetry C35 ERG (=electroretinographic) perimetry C36 VER (= visual evoked response) perimetry C37 EEG (= electroencephalographic) perimetry C38 Light source C39 Daylight# C40 Incandescent lamp C41 Projector lamp C42 Halogen lamp C43 Fluorescent lamp C44 Electronic flash tube C45 Light emitting diode C46 Arc lamp C47 Xenon arc C48 Laser C49 Point-source C50 prefocussed C51 clear C52 frosted C53 Filament C54 Vacillation C55 Ageing C56 Life of a lamp C57 Light housing C58 Reflector C59 Cut-off C60 Projector C61 Dimmer C62 Shutter C63 Screen C64 Diaphragm C65 Ground glass C66 Opal glass C67 Mirror C68 Semitransparent mirror C69 Neutral density filter C70 Neutral density wedge C71 Neutral step density filter C72 Luminance scale C73 Luminance step C74 Colour(or) filter C75 Complementary filter C76 Interference filter C77 Heat absorbing filter C78 Polarizing filter C79 Nicol prism C80 Polaroid C81 transparent C82 translucent C83 opaque C84 Projection obliquity C85 Zoom magnification system C86 Calibration C87 Photometric control C88 Radiometer C89 Spectroradiometer C90 Photometer C91 Spectrophotometer C92 Photocell C93 Photomultiplier C94 Luminance meter C95 Luxmeter C96 Standard of light C97 Comparison surface C98 Discolouration (UK), discoloration (US) C99 Yellowing C100 Smudging C101 Ametropia C102 Empty field myopia C103 Night myopia C104 Instrument myopia C105 Presbyopia C106 Cycloplegia C107 Resting point of accommodation C108 Optical correction C109 Spectacles C110 Correction lens C111 Correction lens holder C112 Tinted lens C113 Contact lens (hard, soft, flexible, corneal, scleral) C114 Eikonic lens C115 Achromatizing lens C116 Blinking C117 Natural pupil C118 Pupillometer C119 Artificial pupil C120 Maxwellian view C121 Stabilized retinal image C122 Chin rest C123 Forehead rest C124 Dental bite bar C125 Occluder C126 Fixation device C127 Fixation control C128 Infrared image converter C129 Visual fixation control C130 Electronic fixed monitor C131 Isopter perimetry C132 Profile perimetry C133 Meridional perimetry C134 Circular perimetry C135 Kinetic perimetry C136 centripetal C137 centrifugal C138 clockwise C139 anticlockwise (UK), counter-clockwise (US) C140 Rate of movement C141 Automatic object translation C142 Static perimetry C143 Single stimulus C144 Multiple stimuli C145 Multiple pattern C146 Frequency of presentation C147 Flash C148 Tachistoscopic presentation C149 Kinetic-static perimetry C150 Perimetrist C151 Subject (=observer, =patient) C152 experienced C153 inexperienced C154 Ascending method of limits C155 Descending method of limits C156 Frequency-of-seeing curve C157 Forced binary choice C158 Forced multiple choice C159 Subject's response criterion C160 Judgement time C161 (Degree of) Coopration of the subject C162 Motivation C163 Reaction time C164 Chronometer C165 Test period C166 Rest period C167 Accommodation C168 Relaxation of accommodation C169 Spiral shaped pattern C170 Star shaped pattern C171 Repeat static test C172 Extinction phenomenon C173 False positive response C174 False negative response C175 Delayed response C176 Signal device C177 Verbal response C178 Manual response C179 Push-button C180 Buzzer C181 Manual recording C182 Semi-automatic recording C183 Computerised perimetry C184 Automation C185 Programme (UK), Program (US) C186 Computerised perimetry C187 Chart C188 Chart scale C189 Polygonal connection (of isopter) C190 Fluent fitting (of isopter) by eye C191 Cartographic deformation C192 Polar azimuthal equidistant projection C193 Central tangential projection C194 Parabolic projection C195 Equivalent projections C196 Conformal projections C197 To hatch C198 Interpretation of a visual field chart C199 Area of a field defect C200 Density of a field defect C201 Protocol C202 Control examination C203 Follow-up C204 Data bank D0 Normal Visual Field D1 Ergoramic occupational visual field D2 Panoramic occupational visual field D3 Total dynamic field D4 Fixation point D5 Meridian D6 Parallel circle D7 Central field D8 Midzone D9 Periphery D10 Hemifield D11 Quadrant D12 temporal D13 nasal D14 superior D15 inferior D16 supero-temporal, etc D17 Eccentricity D18 Absolute limits D19 Profile D20 Isopter D21 Central peak D22 Peripheral limits D23 Blind spot D24 Angioscotoma D25 Vertical symmetry of the isopters D26 Central scotoma at low light levels D27 Hemiopic border D28 Horizontal raphe D29 Refractive scotoma D30 Rotation of the blind spot E0 Pathology E1 Defect E2 Absolute defect E3 Relative defect E4 Chromatic defect E5 Gradient E6 Steep slope E7 Gradual (=gentle) slope E8 Notch E9 Peripheral defect E10 Contraction E11 Concentric contraction E12 Generalized concentric contraction E13 Scotoma E14 Positive scotoma E15 Negative scotoma E16 Depression of the sensitivity curve E17 Central scotoma E18 Eccentric fixation E19 Eccentric viewing E20 Displacement of the blind spot E21 Macular scotoma E22 Scotoma caused by inhibition E23 Paracentral scotoma E24 Pericentral scotoma E25 Paracaecal scotoma E26 Pericaecal scotoma (=enlargement of the blind spot) E27 Baring of the blind spot E28 Centrocaecal scotoma E29 Ring scotoma E30 Zonular scotoma E31 Nerve fibre(er)s bundle defect (=NFBD) E32 Central NFBD E33 Juxta-papillary NFBD E34 Arcuate NFBD (=Bjerrum scotoma) E35 A NFBD in a nasal quadrant E36 A NFBD in a temporal quadrant E37 Cuneate NFBD E38 NFBD proceeding away from the blind spot E39 NFBD proceeding towards the blind spot E40 Break through E41 Swiss cheese defect, Sieve-like defect E42 Defect of vascular origin E43 Neuroscotoma E44 Hemianopia E45 hemianopic E46 Hemidysopia (=relative hemianopia) E47 Hemiachromatopsia E48 Quadrantanopia E49 quadrantic E50 Quadrant dysopia (=relative quadrantanopia) E51 heteronymous E52 bitemporal E53 binasal E54 homonymous E55 left E56 right E57 vertical E58 horizontal E59 crossed E60 Step (nasal etc) E61 hemianopic central scotoma (heteronymous-, etc) E62 Quadrantanopic central scotoma (id) E63 Symmetrical defect E64 Asymmetrical defect E65 Congruent defect E66 Incongruent defect E67 Temporal crescent E68 Overshot E69 Sparing of the macula E70 Splitting of the macula E71 Agnosia E72 Cortical blindness E73 Handicap E74 Degree of disability E75 one-eyed E76 Esterman grid E77 Enlargement of a field defect E78 Diminution of a field defect E79 Disappearance of a field defect E80 Malingering (or simulation) of a field defect E81 Hysterical field defect E82 Concealment of a field defect Copyright 2008. Imaging and Perimetry Society |